Physical exercise induces mental flow related to catecholamine levels in noncompetitive, but not competitive conditions in men
Scientific Reports volume 13, Article number: 14238 (2023) Cite this article
25 Accesses
1 Altmetric
Metrics details
The study aimed to reveal physical exercise conditions and catecholamine response-dependent differences while an individual experiences a flow state (FS) following noncompetitive and competitive running drills. Urine laboratory catecholamine levels were measured using a standard clinical method during pre- and post-physical exercises. The noncompetitive task involved intermittent running drills, from an absolute beginning up through exhaustion. Initially, the drill is performed individually then later competing alongside other runners. Twenty-two males (mean age: 40.27; SD: 5.4; min–max: 31–49 years) were selected in accordance to the following criterion: healthy status without using medication, routine forms of training (running, cycling or swimming) ideally performed with regularity, at least three times per week, 45 min per session. During the noncompetitive task, a high FS experience was associated with a low level of catecholamines, (noradrenaline and adrenaline) while in parallel, the high FS was associated with a low concentration of homovallinic acid. During competitive conditions, the FS-related catecholamine level changes have not yet been found. In conclusion, the low concentration of the circulating catecholamines supports the transient hypofrontality hypothesis regarding the FS experiences. Furthermore, synchronized noradrenaline and adrenaline neurosecretion play an essential role in the manifestation and the prolongation of FS in noncompetitive exercise conditions.
According to Nakamura et al., the flow state manifests as an unexpectedly euphoric, uplifting sensation in which a distal environmental event fuses with an individual’s body and personal space. The spatial and temporal cues of the currently executed actions withdraw from the foreground regarding conscious control ushering to a place consumed with an automatic sequence of successive actions. They flow without strong attention control, worrying, and unpleasant efforts1. Attention does not respond to environmental cues, and the social demands become marginal, however, realization remains goal-oriented and self-confident2,3,4. The general description of FS contains a positive affective state involving high cognitive flexibility to integrate complex visual scenes and motor actions. The initial starting point of the flow-guided action is a confidently running mental schema. In most cases, the FS manifests in conditions associated with high-level mental or physical performances and intensive physiological reactions while the outcome of the flow-guided behavior remains individually positively evaluated5. The flow state definition originates from several interviews among artists, athletes, outdoor enthusiasts, scientists, explorers, and drug-dependent populations. Its factors were measured by self-reported questionnaires. The challenge-skills balance, action-awareness merging, clear goals, concentration on the task execution, withdrawn self-consciousness, experiences of intrinsic reward, and independence in the sense and interpretation of the emotional state are the main characteristics of the FS. The FS features a dual nature; one side is considered a cognitive working model, and the other side is a desired affective state, the flow experience per se1.
Under fMRI recording, increased levels of FS were associated with increased activity in the inferior frontal gyrus and putamen, and in contrast, decreased activity was detected in the medial frontal cortex and amygdala6. Similar results were reported in EEG, transcranial direct current brain stimulation, and near-infrared spectroscopy studies7,8,9. Additionally, FS was associated with higher theta activities in the frontal brain areas10. These results support the transient hypofrontality neurocognitive theory (THT), in which FS was introduced by Dietrich11. Due to the current evidence in support of THT, the components of FS can be articulated in a detailed network wherein the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex via the working memory information buffer plays an essential role in the regulation of environmental direct triggers and the affording automatisms from the striatal system. These areas are engaged in the construction of self-reflective references and the conflict-based arousal system of the brain. While individuals maintain their drive toward a goal-directed action, this transient top-down inhibitory system supports them in remaining focused on the task independently from the redundant environmental signals without intensive conscious effort or control12. The FS plays a vast role in innovative artistic creativity including several areas of successful athletic performance. However, rapid changes in the circulation of neurotransmitters and the limitation in fMRI use during intensive physical activity restrict revealing the exact relationship of neurotransmitter background regarding FS.
Human and animal studies demonstrate how physical exercises induce changes in dopamine (D), noradrenaline (NA) and adrenaline (A) while contributing to the form of acute cognitive and behavioral responsibility. The catecholamines, NA, A, and dopamine D are the main regulators in the exercise-induced physical, emotional, and cognitive response and the motivation of the goal-directed execution. The dopaminergic system originates from the substantia nigra and the ventral tegmental nucleus and, via the nigrostriatal pathway, assumes its role in the acquisition and the recalling of the subsequent movement schemata13, 14. On the other hand, in consideration of the dopaminergic mesolimbic-mesocortical pathway connecting with medial prefrontal cortex is engaged with motivation and regulation of the reward-oriented behavior in which the approaching (to do something) and the anticipated reward (to plane and imagine a goal positioned in a spatial and temporal environment context with routes and maps) are concomitant agents15. However, it is worthy to note the component of the catecholamines (CATs) in any case showed a reciprocal regulation from one another depending on the location of the receptors of the brain16. Notably, the conditions of the ongoing exercises influence the outcome of the performance, and the manifestation of the FS7.
The exercises are considered a behavioral intervention which enhances brain health and plasticity while providing neuroprotection and mental and physical fitness. Successful performance demands exact goal description, personal persistence, and excessive training of motor sequences, all of which serve as the main prerequisite to higher-quality sports performance17. A minimal condition during sports activity is to keep the movement and cognitive strategy for automatized control of the action and is the self-confidence and anxiety-free realization of an idea18. Following the consciously controlled movements, drilling the conscious reappraisal training in hypnosis or mindfulness mediations are very beneficial to apply for transmitting the part of the movements into a global gestalt. During the reevaluation, the focus is fixed on the automatic running of the acts while the environmental triggers are desensitized or depersonalized. This mental prolusion plays an essential role in the manifestation of the FS, both in rhythmic, physical drills and the construction of motor sequences. Results from fMRI studies support the specific role of the prefrontal attention network in the induction of FS among athletes and outdoor enthusiasts19. The FS manifests during several types of athletic activities and in both younger and older athletes20. Attention allocation, a relatively effortless deep innovation into the task-related current activity, is the major area in which to understand the FS, the optimal arousal, and athletic performance success21.
Sports and regular physical training make use of the multiple mechanisms of the brain’s hemodynamic and neurotransmitter system, which is beneficial in the enhancement of the motoric skills, social and cognitive capabilities and support the physical and psychic health22,23,24. An earlier study focused on hobby runners found marked catecholamine changes in serum concentration and urine metabolites during physical challenges, namely, the exposition to a noncompetitive or competitive exercise; the D, NA and A concentration remains enhanced until task completion25, 26. The main proposed question is the way in which this catecholamine undergoes change during exercise among individuals with different degrees of FS following the completion of a noncompetitive or competitive physical exercise. While the body and the mental apparatus adapt to the physical challenges, the regulation of the NA, A and D play a pivotal role in the emotional and motivational aspect of the adaptation and influence the integration of affective, cognitive and motoric functions27. The definition of FS, optimal arousal, mental and emotional state to reach excellent physical or mental performance2 has different stages during intensive sport activity. The optimal state is depending on trained motoric and cognitive skills that influence the way of attention allocation. In non-competitive conditions, the dominant mental states are enjoyment, enhanced motivation, perceived control, altered perceptions, absorption, and confidence. However, in a competitive environment the concentration on a fixed goal, the quick decisions, increasing effort, and heightened awareness may be beneficial to reach the win18. In this condition, the race stress behaves as a clutch, that induces a change in the rate of arousal, and modification in the focus of the attention allocation. The present study is focusing on special motoric, emotional and cognitive changes which are subjectively manifested as an intensive FS experience. Considering the Transient Hypofrontality Theory regarding the FS14, the exercise-induced FS may be a manifestation of prefrontal cortex hypoactivity. This suggestion is supported by EEG studies, however, the neurotransmission research, as of yet, has not revealed a coherent concept regarding FS-related CATs associations. The associations between neurotransmitters in the central nervous system and excreted catecholamines in the urine are not fully proven, however there are clinical data that suggests urinary neurotransmitter testing could be a tool to estimate the nervous system function, as in the case of ADHD, PTSD and depression28,29,30. In this study, we suppose excreted CATs (NA, A and D) and their metabolites; Vanillylmandelic acid (VMA) and homovanillic acid (HVA) are associated with the degree of FS which may be manifested both in noncompetitive exercise and competitive exercise. On the other hand, a manifestation of FS varies in rate and patterns dependent upon other personality predispositions including levels of anxiety, the degree of self-confidence, emotional intelligence31, and absorption capability-related parasympathetic psychophysiological activation32. The FS and the excreted CATs associations will be measured in pre-and post-exercise in noncompetitive and competitive running conditions. The introduction of the condition-dependent flow is grounded in Csikszentmihalyi’s3 and Rakei’s et al.31 suggestion in which levels of elevated stress, such as competition or anxiety, results in elevated attention to the environmental demand33 and in parallel, attenuates the emergence regarding FS experiences. We suppose (Hy1): The high score in FS will be higher in noncompetitive than when compared with competitive running conditions. (Hy2): The high score in the FS may be attributed with lower scores in anxiety and higher scores in self-confidence. Furthermore, (Hy3): as a previous study suggested26, the circulated amount of the CATs is associated with the demand for physical effort during exercise in noncompetitive and competitive conditions and will be increased to the end of the task completion, independently from the applied different conditions. The last yet not least hypothesis (Hy4): we suppose the amount of the excreted CATs will be lower in non-competitive than when compared with competitive conditions.
The measured blood parameters did not show significant correlation between the non-competitive and the competitive running test as published before26.
The obtained data alludes to a difference with a large effect size between noncompetitive and competitive conditions in reference to the action-awareness merging (AAM) flow scale. In competitive conditions, the score of the AAM flow was lower than in the noncompetitive condition (Table 1).
The well-being (mean = 11.47; SD = 3.0), depression (mean = 11.28; SD = 2.4), and perceived life stress (mean = 20.1; SD = 5.9) personality traits compared to the mean scores of a sample of the national population indicated these participants could be considered a healthy sample34. Furthermore, higher FS were associated with lower perceived life stress. Moreover, the post-running administered self-reported anxiousness in most of the cases was lower in both noncompetitive and competitive conditions among participants with high flow state experiences. Considering the post-running emotional state, the low somatic anxiety and the high self-confidence was associated with a higher flow state in noncompetitive and competitive situations (Table 2).
Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to test the effect of time (pre- vs. post-task), condition (noncompetitive vs. competitive), and their interaction (time × condition) on individual and summarized catecholamine levels. Results are presented in Table 4 (for Ms and SDs see Table 3; for the visualization of individual data see Supplement/Fig. 1). In the case of NA and NA + A, a significant main effect of time and a significant time × condition interaction effect were found. This implies the level of NA and NA + A increased from pre-task to post-task, however, the increase was significantly higher in the competitive condition. In the case of A and D, a significant main effect of time was detected. Levels of both A and D increased from pre-task to post-task. In consideration of the combined measures, a significant main effect of time and a significant time × condition interaction effect were found regarding the combined level of A and NA. The significant main effect of time implied a general increase in the combined level of A and NA from pre-task to post-task. However, the significant interaction effect showed this increase was significantly greater in the case of the competitive condition, as when compared to the noncompetitive condition.
Concerning the combined level of CATs (Table 4), a significant main effect of time was found. This showed the combined level of A, NA and D increased from pre-task to post-task. In regard to VMA and HVA, a significant main effect of time and the main effect of the condition were found without distinctive interaction between them. In the case of VMA, this meant its levels were higher in the competitive condition, as compared to the noncompetitive condition, and its levels showed a significant increase from pre-task to post-task. Partial η2 values indicated large effect sizes with regard to all significant results.
Levels of HVA were also higher in the competitive condition, as compared to the noncompetitive condition, however, its levels generally showed a significant decrease from pre-task to post-task. Similar data were reported in a previous study by Nagy et al.26.
Pearson’s correlations (Table 5). demonstrated in the noncompetitive conditions that levels of A and NA and also the summarized amount of the A and NA (but not D) was negatively correlated with reported flow state (total score). Similarly, low levels of HVA (D metabolite) were associated with elevated flow state scores in noncompetitive conditions. VMA showed no significant correlation with flow state scores. In competitive conditions, no significant association was found between flow state experience and catecholamines.
This study aimed to explore the relationship between the flow state experience and the excreted catecholamines while participants underwent noncompetitive or competitive standard running tasks. The obtained results indicate that FS manifests both in noncompetitive and competitive tasks yet differs to varying degrees. In comparing noncompetitive vs competitive intermitted running tasks, the degree of the FS and challenges-skills balance CSB scales have not shown any measurable difference. However, the AAM flow state experience showed a lower score in competitive conditions. Interestingly, the obtained results support the Hy1, in which the competitive situation and the manifestation of FS, namely the action-awareness merge (AAM) cognitive function is partially inhibited. This result is consonant with earlier findings regarding stress situations (competition) in which the flow in most cases is lower than when compared with anxiety-free conditions3. The lower flow in competitive exercise is not inevitable.
Among athletes, mainly during elite sports training, successfully applied methods include hypnosis, meditation, mindfulness, and relaxation techniques to improve the task focusing abilities and also to keep calm during periods of stress in an anxiety-provoking situation. This intervention enhances the chance of the FS while parallels the improvement of task performance and enhancement regarding the FS18, 19, 35, 36.
In another course of results, the association analysis between the FS and health-related trait variables revealed FS is associated with low-level general life stress. In examining the relationships between the degree of the FS and positive and negative affective states in post-measured noncompetitive and competitive conditions, the obtained results illustrated both in noncompetitive and competitive conditions, the FS and levels of self-confidence are equally high. On the other side, the high FS is accompanied with a lower level of somatic and in a partially cognitive anxiety state. The FS and state anxiety associations support the Hy2 and previous data1, 3, 33. These correlations indicate anxiety plays a negative while self-confidence play a positive role in the manifestation of the FS. Therefore, anxiety is disadvantageous, however, self-confidence bears an advantageous effect regarding flow embodiment.
The change in pre-and-post-measured CATs in the noncompetitive and competitive conditions is identical to previously reported data26 and show how in all conditions, the pre-and post-measured degree of the CATs concluding task completion significantly increased. The time and condition interaction indicate both NA and A play a vast role in exercise-induced neurotransmitters response. These are supporting results regarding Hy3.
Considering the urinary collected metabolites, it can be stated, changes regarding VMA showed a consonant enhancing pattern, both in noncompetitive and competitive situations. This results in support by others25, however, presently, the nature of the uncontrolled sex-dependent reaction and other cardiovascular and glucose metabolite effects of these systematic changes has not been exactly interpreted.
Distinctively, the HVA concentration was attenuated after performing noncompetitive and competitive conditions. The role of the manifestation of HVA is thought to be a peripheral indicator regarding the central dopaminergic activity37 which significantly decreases following mental stress38, however, in this population, considering the small number of enrolled participants precludes thoroughly investigating mental stress. Seemingly, our data indicates exercise-induced changes in the urine concentration of the CATs, with the exception of HVA, are working in a unidirectional and synchronized means. Clearly, the HVA depicted a reverted line.
The main aim of this study was to reveal the associations between CATs and the intensity of FS intensity. The conducted analyses indicated in noncompetitive conditions, the lower rate of NA and A, and HVA low concentration in urine samples predicts elevated FS systematically associated with challenge-skill balance and action-awareness merging cognitive functions. A similar association in competitive conditions occurs, in which elevated environmental triggered stress has not yet been detected. This data is aligned with our Hy4 which indicates the elevated stress attenuates the manifestation of the FS and inhibits the synchronized release of CATs.
Notably, an result obtained by ANOVA revealed NA + A is higher in competitive conditions. We can underline the importance of this data. We suppose the higher NA + A together may be an essential role in the inhibition of the manifestation of FS in competitive conditions. This difference has been demonstrated during the control of the Hy1.
The interaction of FS and CATs changing follow diurnal rhythms, situation-specific effects, sex, illness- and stress-dependent and their effect are different in the cortical and mesolimbic areas of the brain39. The present research focused on mainly urine samples among healthy males, who underwent different exercise conditions in which trait and state biochemical and behavioral and personality traits were assessed. The CATs sample taking, and the personality stated data, were precisely scheduled early in the evening including all participants. Consequently, the collected data defined in this standard environment and timescale intervals describes characteristics in reference to both the humoral and the psychological state. The neurophysiologic signature of FS as a psychobiological phenomenon is presently developing and raises numerous questions regarding neurocognitive examinations40. The discussion in the present context focuses on the explication of the correlative data since the data gathering in this domain is of yet, considerably limited. Competition in front of an audience triggers elevated effort stress which influences performance and potentially alters personal experiences during an ongoing task. Earlier studies showed the intensity of the FS is inhibited in conditions in which negative emotions are present, in most cases, the pressure of the excessive achievement distracts the control of the allocation of attention. Consequently, the attention focus turns from inner actions schemata and instead remains fixed on the environmental requirements and events3.
As a result, personal achievement including goals are interrupted. The biopsychological examination regarding this attention process evolved into a different yet partially fitting theory in support of the neuropsychological mechanism of the FS. The transient hypofrontality theory (THT)14, implies exercise induces a schema-like regulation unit in which the motor pattern, the sensory inputs, the cognitive design and the autonomic regulation by the intensive activity of the working memory are collected into an integrated schema. These mechanisms reflect the “second nature or deep structure” regarding human cognition and behavior, wherein the goal-directed action is running with low self-awareness, of which, is a beneficial condition for the manifestation of the FS. Other research results provide a detailed description of a variation in the biochemical mechanism of this prefrontal hypofunction manifested with a low serum concentration of CATs. It has been suggested that a routine induces an attenuation in the concentration of NA and A at the same time, the prefrontal activation after finishing the schema configuration is decreased41, 42. Our results support this theoretical context and are consonant with the basic idea of the THT hypothesis.
Transient hypofrontality could be a decreasing ability to perceive the risk or identify the negative consequences of the ongoing flow-associated behavior. A higher degree in flow state experiences detected during non-competitive conditions results in challenges in which skills balance activity is dominant. This is a preparatory mental schema forming the training phase of the planned action and has been running at different times and conditions. To paraphrase, it has mentally been executed before it has been implemented in a defined environmental context. In the course of the schema construction, the schema becomes an abstract “blueprint” for execution. Consequently, the implementation automatically runs down if the environmental cues and conditions are available. In this case, an inner motivated scheme is adjusted to a well-defined environmental condition in which the agent is confident and focuses on conducting and integrating a trained design and an environmental demand. This process is defined as a FS1 wherein the intrinsic reward is the free running of the constructed schema with an adequate adjustment in a real environment.
The relevance of the low CATs level of urine in the manifestation of the FS appears in our results from the noncompetitive condition. We suggest low circulation catecholamines (CATs) are associated with an intrinsic motivation state wherein a self-relevant, well-laid mental schema fits the environmental challenges. In contrast, during competitive conditions in which an individual’s attention focuses on the environmental challenges, the rivals and to win accolades, the noradrenaline and adrenaline-releasing augment and the FS becomes, in part, inhibited. Parallel with this is the synergistic release of CATs, of which, is disrupted. Our data emphasizes how FS may be manifested in a competitive situation as well, however, its embodiment is dependent upon decreasing the amount of the excretion of NA + A. The intervention which decreases the CATs disruption and the level of NA + A neurotransmitters may originate from a psychological source, such as induced by mindfulness and relaxation techniques or other mental training methods deemed advantageous for the generation of the elevated FS experiences.
In consideration of the above-mentioned interactions between several components of CATs and the limited amount of association with an exercise-induced FS, the hypofrontality hypothesis is partially supported. However, the description of the exact nature of interactions is not yet clearly articulated nor fleshed out.
Required sample size for repeated measures ANOVAs was calculated using G*Power 3.143 Setting the threshold for rejecting the null hypothesis to α = 0.05, a sample size of at least 23 participants was calculated to detect even small effects (partial η2 = 0.1) with an adequate power of 0.85. During the preselection phase of the investigation, from a group of recreational athletes, volunteers were invited to participate in a physical training-related experiment. The participant’s current health status was screened using the hemodynamic labor assessment and was duly performed by a Doctor of Medicine. ECG and blood pressure were monitored, and heart rhythm detector was implemented to explore potential risk markers for cardiovascular stress. Furthermore, a psychical health-related questionnaire packet was administered regarding depression, perceived life stress and general health status. All participants’ health-related scores remained in the normal range of the normative population sample. Hemodynamic risks were not revealed. Twenty-two males (mean age: 40.27; SD: 5.4; min–max: 31–49 years, body heigh: 178.14; SD: 5.27; min–max: 170–189 cm; body mass: 82.6, SD: 10.11; min–max: 68.8–110 kg; BMI: 25.66; SD: 2.6; min–max: 21.5–30.8 kg/m2) were recruited for the study, of whom, were selected based on routine forms of exercise (running, cycling or swimming) at least three times per week for a minimum of 45 min per session. Additional criteria included a healthy status (no known acute or chronic disease) and no use of prescription medications. To ensure homogeneity of the group and considering many gender differences, including hormonal system that may affect the outcome, our study was restricted to men. All volunteers were asked to avoid consumption of hormone-containing foods, avoid alcohol and the use of drugs 24 h prior to conducting the experiment. The participants were asked to restrain from exercising during the day prior to their assessment and only water consumption was allowed. All running tests were executed during the evening hours. Questionnaires were also completed within 15 min prior to and immediately following the running tests. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects. The investigation was a part of a larger set of experiments that approved by the Regional Committee for Research Ethics of the Locale State University (ref. No.: 7162/2018). All methods were performed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, the relevant guidelines and regulations.
The blood and urine laboratory health indicators were measured using standardized clinical method pre- and post-noncompetitive and competitive physical exercises. The noncompetitive task involved a single intermittent indoor run, from an initial starting point up through complete exhaustion. The competitive task contained the same exercise, however, the running was conducted in a competitive environment including other runners and it was hosted in front of an audience. Neither the baseline nor pre- and post-conditions exercise and the health state-related anamnesis did not show illness-related deficiencies among the participants. The heart rate was registered in the pre- and post-phase of the conditions by a Polar Team Pro System. Volunteers were fitted with transmitters positioned just beneath the xiphoid. Blood and urinary samples were taken immediately in both pre- and post-conditions while completing noncompetitive and competitive tasks. The procedure was identical to one used in an earlier study26. All exercises were performed in the evening approximately at the same time in all cases. It took on average 15 min.
The sample taking and evaluation follow a standardized method used systematically throughout our investigations. Prior to and following the running tests within a few minutes, venous blood collections were performed and stored in suitable vacutainers for testing cellular blood parameters, plasma glucose, lactate analysis and routine laboratory blood tests. The subjects were in sitting position while the blood sampling was taken. Plasma and serum parameters were measured as published before26.
Prior to and following the running test, middle stream urine was collected and stored in native vials. All samples were checked using a rapid test (Cybow 10) and were transferred to the laboratory where it were aliquoted and frozen at − 80 ° until further needed. Following thaw, catecholamines and metabolites (D, A, NA, VMA and HVA) levels were detected by Shimadzu Prominence High-Performance Liquid Chromatography (HPLC) system with an Antec Decade SDCTM electrochemical detector. Chromsystems® kit (Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, Catecholamines in urine—HPLC kit (ref. No.: 6000)) and reverse phase column (Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, ref. No.: 5100) were used in full compliance to the manufacturer’s recommendations.
VMA and HVA were detected by the same system using Chromsystems® kit (Chromsystems® from ABL&E-JASCO Hungary, Budapest, VMA, HVA, 5-OHIAA in urine—HPLC kit (ref. No.: 1000/B)) and reverse-phase column (ref. No.: 1100/B) in full adherence to the manufacturer’s recommendations. All data were interpreted utilizing the LabSolution program.
The Flow State Questionnaire44 was established by the phenomenological definition of FS1 and contains twenty items including a response sheet with a five-point Likert scale, which ranges from strongly disagree (1), to strongly agree (5). Following the noncompetitive and competitive running tasks, participants were asked to annotate their flow experiences using the response scales. Example for items: “I was absorbed with the task”. “I felt control in the use of my hand.” The total FS score involves two basic elements: the challenges-skills balance (CSB) (Cronbach’s α = 0.921) and the absorptions regarding the task which is the action-awareness merging (AAM) (Cronbach’s α = 0.907) indicating high FS capability in the defined situation. The CSB grounded a stable self-coherence and self-knowledge wherein the individuals possess clear ideas regarding their talents and skills. The motivation to cope with challenges are poised and aligned to their skills. The AAM refers to changes in time and space experiences, the feeling of the absorption of the task completion and the self-perception, automatisms and the diminishing loss in self-control. The internal consistency in the present sample: Cronbach’s α CSB = 0.893; AAM α = 0.833; FS total α = 0.890.
Other personality trait and state assessment methods are the WHO-5 Well-Being Scale45, 46 involving five aspects which measure the degree of general social, psychical, and physical health Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.793). Beck Depression Inventory BDI Short form-H47, 48, contains nine characteristics to measure the level of depression (Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.745). Furthermore, the Perceived Stress Scale49, 50 was used which contains fourteen aspects to assess the degree of perceived life stress in the recent past (Cronbach’ ɑ = 0.859). Post-Race Questionnaire PRQ51, 52 contains eleven items to assess the somatic anxiety, the cognitive anxiety and the current state of self-confidence pre- and post- measurement of athletic forms of exercise (Cronbach’s ɑ = 0.736); somatic anxiety, cognitive anxiety, and self-confidence ɑ = 0.912–0.656.
The Multi-Stage 20-m Shuttle Run test (Beep test) was used as a standardized exercise in our study53, 54. Running induces an exact, well-documented catecholamine secretion which takes part in the regulation of physiological, behavioral and psychological outcomes regarding provoked stress. The concentration of catecholamines in the plasma or their metabolites in urine is a strong indicator of the individual’s stress sensitivity among trained and untrained individuals including animals55, 56. Previous research has documented increasing levels of NA and A are involved in the emotional factor regarding social behavior57. Furthermore, NA is considered a dominant modulator in social behavior, and exercise also induces stress linked to D functions58.
Means and standard deviations were used to describe variables. Levels of flow experience were compared with paired sampled t-tests across varied conditions. Associations between measured and computed trait and state variables were tested using Pearson’s correlations. Repeated measures ANOVAs were used to test the effect of time (pre- vs- post-task), condition (non-competitive vs. competitive), and their interaction on measured and summed values of catecholamine levels. Partial eta-squared (η2) values were computed as measures of effect size. According to Cohen, benchmarks for small, medium, and large effects were set at 0.01, 0.06, 0.14, respectively59. Lastly, Pearson’s correlations were used again to test the association between reported flow experience and levels of catecholamines after the non-competitive condition. SPSS version 22.0 was used for all statistical analyses.
This interpretation remains considerably open to potential questions regarding the role of the D, of which, in association with the CATs, is not yet clearly defined. The role of the HVA during the manifestation of the flow experiences impacts the amount of the metabolite of D, however, this process is not visible. The hemodynamic stress response has not been controlled and we plan to do so, in the upcoming phase of this study. In addition, the present experimental data only represent population of men, thus further studies may be necessary for women.
Data are available at the OSF repository (https://bit.ly/exercise_flow).
Nakamura, J. C., M. . inOxford Handbook of Positive Psychology (ed C. R. Snyder and S. J. Lopez) 89-105 (Oxford University Press, 2009).
Csikszentmihalyi, M. Flow: The Psychology of Optimal Experience 1st edn. (Harper & Row, 1990).
Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. Beyond Boredom and Anxiety. 25th anniversary edn, (Jossey-Bass Publishers, 2000).
Fullagar, C. J., Knight, P. A. & Sovern, H. S. Challenge/skill balance, flow, and performance anxiety. Appl. Psychol.-Int. Rev. 62, 236–259. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.2012.00494.x (2013).
Article Google Scholar
Oláh, A. Anxiety, Coping, and Flow: Empirical Studies in Interactional Perspective (Trefort Kiadó, 2005).
Google Scholar
Ulrich, M., Keller, J., Hoenig, K., Waller, C. & Gron, G. Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. Neuroimage 86, 194–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.019 (2014).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Gold, J. & Ciorciari, J. A transcranial stimulation intervention to support flow state induction. Front Hum. Neurosci. 13, 274. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2019.00274 (2019).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Hirao, K. Prefrontal hemodynamic responses and the degree of flow experience among occupational therapy students during their performance of a cognitive task. J. Educ. Eval. Health Prof. 11, 24. https://doi.org/10.3352/jeehp.2014.11.24 (2014).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Soltész, P., Magyaródi, T., Mózes, T., Nagy, H. & Oláh, A. The electrophysiology of flow. Hungar. Rev. Psychol. 67, 77–103. https://doi.org/10.1556/mpszle.67.2012.1.6 (2012).
Article Google Scholar
Katahira, K. et al. EEG correlates of the flow state: A combination of increased frontal theta and moderate frontocentral alpha rhythm in the mental arithmetic task. Front Psychol. 9, 300. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00300 (2018).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Dietrich, A. Neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the experience of flow. Conscious Cogn. 13, 746–761. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2004.07.002 (2004).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Gaspelin, N. & Luck, S. J. The role of inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends Cogn. Sci. 22, 79–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001 (2018).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Basso, J. C. & Suzuki, W. A. The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology, and neurochemical pathways: A review. Brain Plast. 2, 127–152. https://doi.org/10.3233/BPL-160040 (2017).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Dietrich, A. Transient hypofrontality as a mechanism for the psychological effects of exercise. Psychiat Res. 145, 79–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.07.033 (2006).
Article Google Scholar
Cerpa, J. C., Coutureau, E. & Parkes, S. L. Dopamine and noradrenaline modulation of goal-directed behavior in orbital and medial prefrontal cortex: Toward a division of labor?. Behav. Neurosci. 135, 138–153. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000426 (2021).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
De Deurwaerdere, P., Moison, D., Navailles, S., Porras, G. & Spampinato, U. Regionally and functionally distinct serotonin3 receptors control in vivo dopamine outflow in the rat nucleus accumbens. J. Neurochem. 94, 140–149. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471-4159.2005.03174.x (2005).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Hansenne, M. et al. Further evidence on the relationship between dopamine and novelty seeking: A neuroendocrine study. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 33, 967–977 (2002).
Article Google Scholar
Swann, C., Crust, L. & Vella, S. A. New directions in the psychology of optimal performance in sport: Flow and clutch states. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 16, 48–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.03.032 (2017).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Lutz, A. et al. Mental training enhances attentional stability: Neural and behavioral evidence. J. Neurosci. 29, 13418–13427. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1614-09.2009 (2009).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W. & Smethurst, C. J. Relationships between flow, self-concept, psychological skills, and performance. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 13, 129–153. https://doi.org/10.1080/104132001753149865 (2001).
Article Google Scholar
Harris, D. J., Vine, S. J. & Wilson, M. R. Neurocognitive mechanisms of the flow state. Prog. Brain Res. 234, 221–243. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2017.06.012 (2017).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Konchel, C. et al. Cognitive and behavioral effects of physical exercise in psychiatric patients. Prog. Neurobiol. 96, 46–68 (2012).
Article Google Scholar
Meeusen, R. Exercise and the brain: insight in new therapeutic modalities. Ann. Transplant 10, 49–51 (2005).
PubMed Google Scholar
Radak, Z. et al. Exercise plays a preventive role against Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 20, 777–783. https://doi.org/10.3233/JAD-2010-091531 (2010).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Zouhal, H., Jacob, C., Delamarche, P. & Gratas-Delamarche, A. Catecholamines and the effects of exercise, training and gender. Sports Med. 38, 401–423. https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200838050-00004 (2008).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Nagy, Z. et al. Reward dependence-moderated noradrenergic and hormonal responses during noncompetitive and competitive physical activities. Front. Behav. Neurosci. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2022.763220 (2022).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Lin, T. W. & Kuo, Y. M. Exercise benefits brain function: the monoamine connection. Brain Sci. 3, 39–53. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3010039 (2013).
Article ADS CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Pan, X., Kaminga, A. C., Wen, S. W. & Liu, A. Catecholamines in post-traumatic stress disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Mol. Neurosci. 11, 450. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnmol.2018.00450 (2018).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Tokunaga, A., Kanazawa, Y. & Kawano, A. Relationship between psychological state and urinary catecholamines and serotonin in 1-month postpartum mothers. J. Jpn. Acad. Midw. 35, 113–121. https://doi.org/10.3418/jjam.JJAM-2021-0011 (2021).
Article Google Scholar
Wijaya, C. S. et al. Differentiating medicated patients suffering from major depressive disorder from healthy controls by spot urine measurement of monoamines and steroid hormones. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 15, 865 (2018).
Article MathSciNet PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Rakei, A., Tan, J. & Bhattacharya, J. Flow in contemporary musicians: Individual differences in flow proneness, anxiety, and emotional intelligence. PLoS ONE 17, e0265936. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0265936 (2022).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Peifer, C., Schulz, A., Schachinger, H., Baumann, N. & Antoni, C. H. The relation of flow-experience and physiological arousal under stress—Can u shape it?. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 53, 62–69. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.01.009 (2014).
Article Google Scholar
Martens, R., Vealey, R. S., & Burton, D. . Competitive anxiety in sport. (Human Kinetics, 1990).
Susánszky, É., Szántó, Zs. Magyar lelkiállapot [Hungarian state of mind.]. (Semmelweis Kiadó [Semmelweis Publisher], 2013).
Pates, J. Hypnosis in the clutch. Biomed. J. Sci. Tech. Res. https://doi.org/10.26717/BJSTR.2019.23.003863 (2019).
Article Google Scholar
Pates, J., Cummings, A. & Maynard, I. The effects of hypnosis on flow states and three-point shooting performance in bastketball players. Sport Psychol. 16, 34–47. https://doi.org/10.1123/tsp.16.1.34 (2002).
Article Google Scholar
Stroe, A. E. et al. Diurnal variation in plasma homovanillic acid: not a renal phenomenon. Biol. Psychiatry 41, 621–623. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(96)00526-4 (1997).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Sumiyoshi, T. et al. Effect of mental stress on plasma homovanillic acid in healthy human subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology 19, 70–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(98)00005-0 (1998).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Goldstein, D. S. Catecholamines 101. Clin. Auton. Res. 20, 331–352. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10286-010-0065-7 (2010).
Article ADS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Cheron, G. How to measure the psychological “Flow”? A neuroscience perspective. Front Psychol. 7, 1823. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01823 (2016).
Article PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Audiffren, M. in Exercise and Cognitive Function Vol. 390 (ed Phillip Tomporowski Terry McMorris, Michel Audiffren) Ch. 1, 3–39 (Wiley, 2009).
McMorris, T. Developing the catecholamines hypothesis for the acute exercise-cognition interaction in humans: Lessons from animal studies. Physiol. Behav. 165, 291–299. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.08.011 (2016).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A. & Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3,1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 1149–1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 (2009).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Magyaródi, T., Nagy, H., Soltész, P., Mózes, T. & Oláh, A. Psychometric properties of a newly established flow state questionnaire. J. Happ. Well-Being 1, 85–96 (2013).
Google Scholar
Bech, P., Olsen, L. R., Kjoller, M. & Rasmussen, N. K. Measuring well-being rather than the absence of distress symptoms: A comparison of the SF-36 Mental Health subscale and the WHO-Five Well-Being Scale. Int. J. Methods Psychiatr. Res. 12, 85–91. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.145 (2003).
Article PubMed Google Scholar
Susánszky, É., Konkolÿ-Thege, B., Stauder, A. & Kopp, M. Validation of the short (5-item) version of the WHO Well-Being Scale based on a Hungarian representative health survey, (Hungarostudy 2002). Mentálhigiéné és Pszichoszomatika 7, 247–255 (2006).
Article Google Scholar
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J. & Erbaugh, J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004 (1961).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Rózsa, S., Szádóczky, E. & Füredi, J. Beck depression questionnaire: A national adaptation. Psychiatr. Hung. 16, 384–402 (2001).
Google Scholar
Cohen, S., Kamarck, T. & Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396 (1983).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Stauder, A. & Konkolÿ-Thege, B. Characteristics of the Hungarian version of the perceived stress scale (PSS). Mentálhigiéné és Pszichoszomatika 7, 203–216 (2006).
Article Google Scholar
Sanderson, F. H. in Anxiety in sports (ed Dieter Hackfort and Charles D. Spielberger) (Hemisphere, 1989).
László, Z. Running Athlete's Competitive Anxiety Is Associated with Their Racing Performance. PhD thesis, Pannon University, Hungary, (2009).
Leger, L. & Gadoury, C. Validity of the 20 m shuttle run test with 1 min stages to predict VO2max in adults. Can. J. Sport Sci. 14, 21–26 (1989).
CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Leger, L. A. & Lambert, J. A maximal multistage 20-m shuttle run test to predict VO2 max. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. Occup. Physiol. 49, 1–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00428958 (1982).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Baker, J. S. & Buchan, D. Metabolic stress and high-intensity exercise. Phys. Med. Rehabil. Res. 2, 1–2. https://doi.org/10.15761/PMRR.1000136 (2017).
Article ADS Google Scholar
Eto, K., Mazilu-Brown, J. K., Henderson-MacLennan, N., Dipple, K. M. & McCabe, E. R. Development of catecholamine and cortisol stress responses in zebrafish. Mol. Genet. Metab. Rep. 1, 373–377. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymgmr.2014.08.003 (2014).
Article CAS PubMed PubMed Central Google Scholar
Åstrand, P.-O. & Kaare, R. Textbook of Work Physiology: Physiological Bases of Exercise (McGraw-Hill, 1986).
Google Scholar
Ratge, D., Gehrke, A., Melzner, I. & Wisser, H. Free and conjugated catecholamines in human plasma during physical exercise. Clin. Exp. Pharmacol. Physiol. 13, 543–553. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1681.1986.tb00937.x (1986).
Article CAS PubMed Google Scholar
Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences 2nd edn. (L. Erlbaum Associates, 1988).
MATH Google Scholar
Download references
We wish to acknowledge and thank all of the participants and colleagues for their contribution, namely Laura Takács, Ildikó Bock-Marquette, MD & Ph.D., István Német, Anikó Lajtai, Ph.D., Heléna Halász and the assistants at the Department of Laboratory Medicine of the University of Pécs.
Open access funding provided by University of Pécs. This study was supported by NKFI 120334; and GINOP-2.3.2-15-2016-00047 Grants.
Sports and Physical Education Center, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
István Karsai
Department of Laboratory Medicine, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
Zsófia Nagy, Tamás Nagy, Emese Kátai & Attila Miseta
Sport and Medicine Research Group, Regenerative Science, Szentágothai Research Center, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
Zsófia Nagy
Institute of Psychology, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
Ferenc Kocsor & András Láng
Department of Vascular Surgery, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
Gábor Fazekas
Department of Behavioral Sciences, Medical School, University of Pécs, Pécs, Hungary
János Kállai
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
I.K.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Writing—supervision. Z.N.: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Resources, Project administration, Writing—original draft, edit, supervision. Corresponding author. T.N.: Writing—edit, proofreading, supervision. E.K.: Investigation, Writing—supervision. A.M.: Writing—supervision. G.F.: Writing—supervision. A.L.: Formal analysis, Writing—edit, supervision. F.K.: Formal analysis, Writing—supervision. J.K.: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Writing—original draft, edit, supervision.
Correspondence to Zsófia Nagy.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and Permissions
Karsai, I., Nagy, Z., Nagy, T. et al. Physical exercise induces mental flow related to catecholamine levels in noncompetitive, but not competitive conditions in men. Sci Rep 13, 14238 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-41518-2
Download citation
Received: 21 March 2023
Accepted: 28 August 2023
Published: 30 August 2023
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-41518-2
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.
Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.